Skip to Main Content

Login to your account

Show
Forgot password?
Don’t have an account?
Create a Free Account

If you don't remember your password, you can reset it by entering your email address and clicking the Reset Password button. You will then receive an email that contains a secure link for resetting your password

If the address matches a valid account an email will be sent to __email__ with instructions for resetting your password

Cancel
ADVERTISEMENT
SCROLL TO CONTINUE WITH CONTENT



Property Value
Status
Version
Ad File
Disable Ads Flag
Environment
Moat Init
Moat Ready
Contextual Ready
Contextual URL
Contextual Initial Segments
Contextual Used Segments
AdUnit
SubAdUnit
Custom Targeting
Ad Events
Invalid Ad Sizes
Advertisement
Journal of Pediatric Nursing
  • Submit
  • Log in
  • Register
  • Log in
    • Submit
    • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • Claim

Access provided by

Skip menu
  • Articles
    • Cover Image - Journal of Pediatric Nursing: Nursing Care of Children and Families, Volume 70, Issue undefined
    • Latest

      • Articles in Press
      • Issues In Progress
      • Current issue
      • Past Issues
    • Popular Articles

      • Society of Pediatric Nurses Department

        Immunizations – Nursing Interventions to Enhance Vaccination Rates

        • Ann M. Bowling
      • Research Article

        The effect of using maternal voice, white noise, and holding combination interventions on the heel stick sampling

          Apaydin Cirik et al.
      • Editorial

        Infants with colic: Their childhood characteristics

          Neu et al.
    • Latest Articles

      • Research Article
        Open Access

        Barriers and facilitators of family rules and routines during pediatric cancer treatment

          Bates et al.
      • Review Article

        Pedagogical practices developed with children through hospital classes: An integrative literature review

          Ludgério et al.
      • Research Article

        Polycystic ovary syndrome and body image concerns during adolescence

          Saei Ghare Naz et al.
  • Publish
    • For Authors

      • Submit Article
        External Link
      • Guide for Authors
      • Aims & Scope
      • Open Access Information
        External Link
      • Researcher Academy
        External Link
  • Topics
    • Collections

      • Covid-19
      • Instruments and Tools Article Collection
  • About
    • Society

      • SPN
        External Link
      • PENS
        External Link
      • Editorial Board
    • Journal Information

      • Aims & Scope
      • Permissions
      • Reprints
        External Link
      • Abstracting & Indexing
    • Access

      • Subscribe
      • Activate Online Access
  • Contact
    • Contact

      • Contact Us
      • Career Opportunities
        External Link
      • Advertise with Us
        External Link
      • Go to Product Catalog
        External Link
    • Follow Us

      • New Content Alerts
Advanced search
Advanced search

Please enter a term before submitting your search.

Ok
Research Article| Volume 65, P108-115, July 2022

Download started.

Ok
  • PDF [836 KB]PDF [836 KB]
  • Figures
    • Figure Viewer
    • Download Figures (PPT)
  • Save
    • Add To Online LibraryPowered ByMendeley
    • Add To My Reading List
    • Export Citation
    • Create Citation Alert
  • Share
    Share on
    • Twitter
    • Facebook
    • Linked In
    • Sina Weibo
    • Email
  • more
    • Reprints
    • Request
  • Top

Psychometric properties of a self-assessment fear scale in children aged 4 to 12 years. Scary Scale

  • S. Thurillet
    S. Thurillet
    Correspondence
    Corresponding author at: Pediatric Nurse, Pediatric Emergency Department, Mother-Child Hospital, 8 avenue Dominique Larrey, 87042 Limoges, Cedex, France.
    Contact
    Affiliations
    Pediatric Emergency Department, Mother-Child Hospital, Limoges, France
    Search for articles by this author
  • C. Bahans
    C. Bahans
      Affiliations
      Pediatric Epidemiology Department, Mother-Child Hospital, Limoges, France
      Search for articles by this author
    • C. Wood
      C. Wood
        Affiliations
        Pain Department, Mother-Child Hospital, Limoges, France
        Search for articles by this author
      • S. Bougnard
        S. Bougnard
          Affiliations
          Pediatric Emergency Department, Mother-Child Hospital, Limoges, France
          Search for articles by this author
        • A. Labrunie
          A. Labrunie
            Affiliations
            Centre of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Methodology in Research, Limoges, France
            Search for articles by this author
          • V. Messager
            V. Messager
              Affiliations
              Pediatric Emergency Department, Mother-Child Hospital, Limoges, France
              Search for articles by this author
            • J. Toniolo
              J. Toniolo
                Affiliations
                University Hospital Centre, Inserm U1094, IRD U270, Univ. Limoges, CHU Limoges, EpiMaCT - Epidemiology of chronic diseases in tropical zone, Institute of Epidemiology and Tropical Neurology, OmegaHealth, Limoges, France
                Search for articles by this author
              • P. Beloni
                P. Beloni
                  Affiliations
                  University Hospital Centre, Inserm U1094, IRD U270, Univ. Limoges, CHU Limoges, EpiMaCT - Epidemiology of chronic diseases in tropical zone, Institute of Epidemiology and Tropical Neurology, OmegaHealth, Limoges, France
                  Search for articles by this author
                • L. Fourcade
                  L. Fourcade
                    Affiliations
                    Pediatric Surgery Department, Mother-Child Hospital, Limoges, France
                    Search for articles by this author
                  Open AccessPublished:March 14, 2022DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2022.02.020
                  Psychometric properties of a self-assessment fear scale in children aged 4 to 12 years. Scary Scale
                  Previous ArticleAttitudes and practices towards vital signs monitoring on paediatric wards: Cross-validation of the Ped-V scale
                  Next ArticleMoving up: Healthcare transition experiences of adolescents and young adults with cystic fibrosis
                      Advertisement

                      Highlights

                      • •
                        Children will be able to describe how they feel about the reason for their fear.
                      • •
                        Our fear scale could be used in all care situations triggering fear.
                      • •
                        Our scale will highlight link between pain and fear in health care situations.
                      • •
                        Caregivers may be able to notice the difference between fear and pain diagnoses.
                      • •
                        Better adaptation of their pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical strategies.

                      Abstract

                      Background

                      Caregivers encounter difficulties differentiating fear and pain experienced by children and tend to interpret what children may feel, often resulting in inadequate pain management. While many pain self-assessment scales are available, there is no validated self-assessment fear scale for children.

                      Methods

                      The aim of this prospective study was to validate, in children aged 4 to 12 years, the psychometric properties of our scale. In a first part, in a school setting, five exercises were given to 484 children in order to validate the expression of fear, grade the intensity of the faces, the ability to discriminate the faces and the equality of the intervals. The scale's reproducibility was studied by assessing the children's fear in everyday situations at two different time points.
                      In a second part, in a hospital setting, the aim was to test the scale's feasibility. Sixty children admitted to one emergency care department self-assessed their fear with the Scary Scale.

                      Findings

                      The expression of fear was validated by 57.64% (p < 0.0001) of the children in comparison with three other emotions (pain, surprise, sadness).The 7–9 year-olds validated the other properties (gradation, discrimination, equality, reproducibility). The 4–6 year-olds failed to validate the gradation exercise, but succeeded with the others.
                      In the hospital, 95% of children self-assessed their fear using the scale.

                      Discussion

                      Our self-assessment fear scale was validated in children aged 7–12 years specifically and was readily feasible in the hospital. We recommend its use in that age group in every care situation triggering fear.
                      Trial registration: clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT02675504.

                      Keywords

                      • Fear
                      • Scale
                      • Self-assessment
                      • Children
                      • Nurse diagnosis

                      Introduction

                      Nurses analyze care situations based on Virginia Henderson's fourteen basic principles (
                      Henderson, 1997
                      • Henderson V.
                      Basic principles of nursing care.
                      American Nurses Association, 1997
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). From these basic principles, the North American Nursing Diagnosis Association-International (NANDA-I) has developed and described nursing diagnoses (
                      Ackley, Ladwig, Makic, Martinez-Kratz and Zanotti, 2019
                      • Ackley B.
                      • Ladwig G.
                      • Makic M.B.
                      • Martinez-Kratz M.
                      • Zanotti M.
                      Nursing diagnosis handbook. An evidence-based guide to planning care.
                      12th éd. Mosby, 2019
                      • Google Scholar
                      ) to assist caregivers in the analysis of clinical situations. Pain and fear are two nursing diagnoses developed from the fundamental principle of “security”.
                      The International Association for the Study of Pain defines pain as an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage (
                      International Association for the Study of Pain, 2022
                      • International Association for the Study of Pain
                      IASP terminology - Pain definition.
                      2022
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). By means of the different assessment scales and available therapeutics, nurses can analyze and act in a manner adapted to the diagnosis of pain.
                      Fear is a distressing emotion caused by an impending danger or pain, whether the threat is real or imagined. Some of the most common fears are fear of death, pain and bodily injury. Some of the most common examples of fear that a nurse can encounter are patients' fears during diagnostic testing. The nurse's role is to identify when patients are experiencing fear by assessing the behavioral and verbal expression of this fear and must find ways to help them in a respectful way to face these feelings (
                      Ackley, Ladwig, Makic, Martinez-Kratz and Zanotti, 2019
                      • Ackley B.
                      • Ladwig G.
                      • Makic M.B.
                      • Martinez-Kratz M.
                      • Zanotti M.
                      Nursing diagnosis handbook. An evidence-based guide to planning care.
                      12th éd. Mosby, 2019
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). However, even though the diagnosis of fear is very well described in terms of evaluation and support, it is little or even not evaluated. A self-assessment fear scale already exists: the Children Fear Scale (
                      McMurtry, Noel, Chambers and McGrath, 2011
                      • McMurtry C.M.
                      • Noel M.
                      • Chambers C.T.
                      • McGrath P.J.
                      Children’s fear during procedural pain: Preliminary investigation of the Children’s fear scale.
                      Health Psychology: Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association. 2011; 30: 780-788
                      https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024817
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (166)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ), though it is under-used due to major limitations, such as a high degree of graphic similarity with the Faces Pain Scale-Revised (FPS-R) (
                      Hicks, von Baeyer, Spafford, van Korlaar, & Goodenough, 2001
                      • Hicks Carrie L
                      • von Baeyer Carl L
                      • Spafford Pamela A
                      • van Korlaar Inez
                      • Goodenough Belinda
                      The Faces Pain Scale-Revised: toward a common metric in pediatric pain measurement.
                      pain. 2001; 93: 173-183https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00314-1
                      • Abstract
                      • Full Text
                      • Full Text PDF
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (1350)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ).
                      Fear and pain are two subjective concepts that involve emotions. Each one influences the other and they can be difficult to distinguish. Their clinical expressions are similar (
                      Binet, 1895
                      • Binet A.
                      Fear in children.
                      Vol. 2. 1895: 223-254
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Dantzer, 1993
                      • Dantzer R.
                      The psychobiology of fear.
                      Communications. 1993; 57: 25-34
                      https://doi.org/10.3406/comm.1993.1864
                      • Crossref
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Garber, Garber and Spizman, 1997
                      • Garber S.
                      • Garber M.
                      • Spizman R.
                      Your child's fears. How to help him/her overcome them.
                      Odile Jacob, 1997
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Pilon, 1999
                      • Pilon C.
                      Relieve the child's pain.
                      Hôpital Sainte Justine, 1999
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). They also activate the same structures in the brain. The affective component of pain activates the limbic structures also involved in fear (
                      Marchand, 1998
                      • Marchand S.
                      The phenomenon of pain. Understanding for better care.
                      Masson, 1998
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). As such, the sensory and affective processes occur in parallel and simultaneously, adding to this confusion between pain and fear.
                      In medical practice, the lack of a clear distinction between these two entities creates difficulties in assessing pain in children and sometimes results in inadequate care. Management of pain does not eliminate fear. By attempting to treat what is actually fear with analgesics, our management is ineffective, whereas there are methods for managing fear in children (information given before the treatment, the examination, or the procedure using playful materials, and distraction/hypnoanalgesia during the treatment). A better distinction between fear and pain will help avoid over-consumption of opioids.
                      The aim of this study was to develop a self-assessment fear scale, called “Scary Scale” and to validate its psychometric properties in children aged from 4 to 12 years. In its first part, the study was conducted in a school setting - a neutral environment – so as to avoid bias in the answers. To test the feasibility of the use of this scale, the second part of the study was carried out under real life conditions, in a hospital setting. We also wanted to see if a concordance existed between children's self-reported fear scores and proxy-reported fear scores by caregivers upon hospital arrival.

                      Methods

                      Rationale for the creation of the scale

                      A self-assessment fear scale, the “Scary Scale”, was designed (Fig. 1), with its characteristics drawn from literature review (
                      Beyer et al., 1992
                      • Beyer Judith E
                      • et al.
                      The creation, validation, and continuing development of the oucher: A measure of pain intensity in children.
                      Journal of Pediatric Nursing: Nursing Care of Children and Families. 1992; 7: 335-346https://doi.org/10.5555/uri:pii:0882596392900426
                      • Abstract
                      • Full Text PDF
                      • PubMed
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Beyer et al., 2005
                      • Beyer J.E.
                      • Turner S.B.
                      • Jones L.
                      • Young L.
                      • Onikul R.
                      • Bohaty B.
                      The alternate forms reliability of the Oucher pain scale.
                      Pain Management Nursing: Official Journal of the American Society of Pain Management Nurses. 2005; 6: 10-17
                      https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmn.2004.11.001
                      • Abstract
                      • Full Text
                      • Full Text PDF
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (33)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Bieri et al., 1990
                      • Bieri D
                      • et al.
                      The Faces Pain Scale for the self-assessment of the severity of pain experienced by children: development, initial validation, and preliminary investigation for ratio scale properties.
                      Pain. 1990; 41: 139-150https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(90)90018-9
                      • Abstract
                      • Full Text PDF
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (875)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Foster and Park, 2012
                      • Foster R.L.
                      • Park J.
                      An integrative review of literature examining psychometric properties of instruments measuring anxiety or fear in hospitalized children.
                      Pain Management Nursing: Official Journal of the American Society of Pain Management Nurses. 2012; 13: 94-106
                      https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmn.2011.06.006
                      • Abstract
                      • Full Text
                      • Full Text PDF
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (33)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Garra et al., 2010
                      • Garra G.
                      • Singer A.J.
                      • Taira B.R.
                      • Chohan J.
                      • Cardoz H.
                      • Chisena E.
                      • Thode H.C.
                      Validation of the Wong-baker FACES pain rating scale in pediatric emergency department patients.
                      Academic Emergency Medicine: Official Journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine. 2010; 17: 50-54
                      https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2009.00620.x
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (326)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Kuttner and Lepage, 1989
                      • Kuttner L.
                      • Lepage T.
                      Face scales for the assessment of pediatric pain: A critical review.
                      Vol. 21(2). 1989: 198-209
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Streiner and Norman, 2003
                      • Streiner D.L.
                      • Norman G.R.
                      Health measurement scales. A practical guide to their development and use.
                      3rd éd. Oxford University Press, 2003
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Von Baeyer, 2006
                      • Von Baeyer C.L.
                      Children’s self-reports of pain intensity: Scale selection, limitations and interpretation.
                      Pain Research & Management: The Journal of the Canadian Pain Society. 2006; 11: 157-162
                      https://doi.org/10.1155/2006/197616
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (300)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Yeh, 2005
                      • Yeh C.-H.
                      Development and validation of the Asian version of the oucher: A pain intensity scale for children.
                      The Journal of Pain: Official Journal of the American Pain Society. 2005; 6: 526-534
                      https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2005.03.002
                      • Abstract
                      • Full Text
                      • Full Text PDF
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (20)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ) and from our own experience.
                      Fig. 1
                      Fig. 1Scary Scale.
                      Show full caption
                      Instructions: These faces show how much fear you can have. This face (show the one on the left) shows someone who has no fear at all. These faces (show them one by one from left to right) show someone who is more and more afraid, up to this one (show the one on the right), that shows someone who is very afraid. Show me the face that shows how afraid you are right now. As soon as the child shows from the second face, make him/her state the object of his fear. It is up to you to give him/her the necessary information to explain his/her healthcare pathway, care, exams, etc., using playful supports before and/or during.
                      • View Large Image
                      • Figure Viewer
                      • Download Hi-res image
                      • Download (PPT)
                      The graphics for our scale were based on the studies of Ekman, who developed the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) (
                      Ekman and Friesen, 2002
                      • Ekman P.
                      • Friesen W.V.
                      Investigator’s guide to the facial action coding system (FACS).
                      2002
                      • Google Scholar
                      ).
                      As there is no gold standard scale in the literature using faces for the self-assessment of fear, we based the method for our study on that of Bieri et al. (Bieri et al., 1990) using the same steps. Unlike Bieri's study, in which children drew the pictures of the Face Pain Scale (FPS), resulting in a scale with seven faces expressing pain, a graphic designer created an original scale with six faces specifically expressing fear.
                      It consists of six faces of gradually intensifying fear. The scale rates fear with a score ranging from 0 to 10 (rated two by two, gradually) that is consistent with pain rating scales.
                      Regarding trait validity and tool design, an assistant professor of clinical psychology specialized in applied developmental emphasis on fear and pain in children in medical context was consulted. Additionally, five pediatric nurses, four pediatricians and a nurse researcher were questioned to validate the scale design.

                      Sample and setting

                      Sampling was performed in two ways relative to the two parts of our study.
                      School population: the study took place in seven schools. The inclusion criteria were schoolchildren aged from 4 to 12 years, with parental consent. The non-inclusion criteria were children with psychomotor and intellectual difficulties that impeded understanding of the instructions and use of the scale.
                      Hospital population: the study took place in an emergency care department. The inclusion criteria were children aged from 4 to 12 years, for whom parental consent was obtained, who were seen during admission in the pediatric emergency care department. The non-inclusion criteria were children admitted in a life-threatening emergency situation, and children with psychomotor and intellectual difficulties that impeded understanding the instructions and use of the scale.

                      Procedure

                      In schools: psychometric assessment. The principal investigator provided training for research caregivers in a standardized manner. Two groups of three caregivers spent 20 min in a class for primary school pupils. One gave instructions and two others monitored the conduct of the study. For kindergarten children, each caregiver explained the exercises individually to the children in turn for 20 min each. The study took place on Tuesday or Thursday mornings, which is a time of day and week when attention and concentration capabilities are at their highest (
                      Testu, Bréchon, Clarisse, Fontaine and Le Floc'h, N., 2008
                      • Testu F.
                      • Bréchon G.
                      • Clarisse R.
                      • Fontaine R.
                      • Le Floc’h, N.
                      Life rhythms and school rhythms.
                      Elsevier Masson, 2008
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). Each child was given a file that included five exercises (phase 1). Details of the five exercises are available in the supplemental file.
                      Construct validity was evaluated with exercises one to four.
                      The objective of exercise 1 was to determine the validity of the expression of fear with the scale to confirm that the faces actually express fear. The scale was presented to the children. It was explained to them that the faces showed someone who felt an emotion growing up. They had to choose the response that expresses this emotion, according to them, among four propositions: fear, sadness, surprise, pain. These emotions were chosen due to their possible confusion. According to Ekman (
                      Ekman and Friesen, 2002
                      • Ekman P.
                      • Friesen W.V.
                      Investigator’s guide to the facial action coding system (FACS).
                      2002
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Ekman and Friesen, 2003
                      • Ekman P.
                      • Friesen W.V.
                      Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial expressions (Malor books).
                      2003
                      • Google Scholar
                      ), fear consists of 6 units of action (Number 1–2–4-5-20-26). Sadness consists of 3 units of action (number 1–4-15). It had 2 out of 3 units in common with those of fear. Surprise consists of 4 units of actions (Number 1–2–5-26). All units are common with those of fear. Pain was chosen because of its behavioral component that is common with fear (cry, scream, wince).
                      The objective of exercise 2 was to rank the intensity gradient in the expression of fear on the six faces.
                      The children were given six shuffled stickers representing the six faces of our scale and were asked to rank them in descending order of fear.
                      The objective of exercise 3 was to compare, two-by-two, the intensity of the expression of fear. The six faces of the scale were separated in order to form pairs. This exercise was presented as a table integrating the fifteen possible pairs of combinations (e.g., face 1 with face 6, face 3 with face 5). Given each pair, the children were then asked to circle the face that expressed the most fear. For the kindergarten children, a cover with lifting windows was made so that the children could focus on one pair of faces at a time.
                      The objective of exercise 4 was to determine whether the position (distance in cm) of each face, starting from face 1,was in the predefined interval (every 5 cm) and to ascertain whether the intensities of the six faces of the fear scale were equal.On the page of this exercise, an enlarged version (25 × 10 cm) of the Visual Analog Scale (VAS) (
                      Huskisson, 1974
                      • Huskisson E.C.
                      Measurement of pain.
                      Lancet (London, England). 1974; 2: 1127-1131
                      https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(74)90884-8
                      • Abstract
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (3163)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ) was presented as an upside-down triangle. The two stickers that expressed the extremes on the fear scale were attached beforehand, with the sticker expressing the absence of fear (score 0) and the sticker expressing the greatest fear possible (score 10). The children were given the four remaining intermediate stickers in a mixed order. The stickers measured 2.5 × 2.5 cm.
                      It was explained that the red triangle represented fear. The face at the very bottom showed the absence of fear. The wider the triangle became, the greater the fear was, up to the very top of the triangle (face expressing the greatest amount of fear). The children had to place the faces in the triangle based on the intensity of fear that each face expressed.
                      Test-retest reliability was evaluated by exercise five.
                      The objective of exercise 5 was to evaluate the reproducibility of the self-assessment fear scale with the help of everyday situations of fear as illustrated in the photographs. Six pictures of real-life situations triggering fear in children (the most frequently cited in the literature being fear of water, dark, needles, dogs, thunderstorms and spiders) (
                      Copper-Royer, 2013
                      • Copper-Royer B.
                      Fear of the wolf, fear of everything.
                      Albin Michel, 2013
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Petitcollin, 2003
                      • Petitcollin C.
                      Emotions, user's guide.
                      Jouvence, 2003
                      • Google Scholar
                      ) were presented to the children. For each situation, the children used the self-assessment scale to rate by themselves, circling the face most closely corresponding to the fear experienced.
                      To determine the reproducibility of the scale, this exercise was repeated 15 days later (phase 2).We asked the children to remember their answers given the first time by evaluating once again their fear with the same pictures.
                      The data from the five exercises were electronically recorded in anonymized form.
                      In the hospital: feasibility assessment.
                      Children were included upon their arrival after consent from their parents.
                      We wanted to determine whether using our scale in a care department would be feasible. We wanted to know whether children were able to rate their fear with the scale. Upon admission, before any care, the Scary Scale was presented to the child to assess their own level of fear. If the child gave a fear score greater than or equal to 2 out of 10, the child had to identify the object of their fear.
                      In order to answer our last objective, the concordance between children's self-reported fear scores and proxy-reported fear scores by two caregivers upon hospital arrival was also evaluated in order to ascertain whether there was a gap between the fear experienced by children and their caregivers' evaluation. For the proxy assessment, at the same time as the child's self-assessment, each of the two caregivers independently assessed the child's level of fear using the Scary Scale (score from 0 to 10).
                      During admission, the two caregivers collected the initial information (circumstances of the admission, hemodynamic parameters and pain assessment with Faces Pain Scale-Revised).
                      All of the data were collected in a paper file using the computer software of the emergency department and were then electronically entered into an anonymized form.

                      Ethics

                      This study was authorized by an Ethics Committee on October 9, 2015 under No CCP15–037 / 2015-A00927–42 and with authorization from the school directors.

                      Data analysis

                      Calculation of the number of subjects needed for the study

                      In the school setting, the number of subjects was calculated based on exercise 4 which needed the highest number of subjects.
                      Concerning the intervals between faces, our hypothesis on the means of the differences between the interval values observed and the theoretical interval values, was based on the study by Bieri et al.(Bieri et al., 1990).Accordingly, we hypothesized four means of differences: a difference of 2.3 cm for the position of face 2, a difference of 6.5 cm for the position of face 3, a difference of 4.6 cm for the position of face 4 and a difference of 6.4 cm for the position of face 5.
                      The common standard deviation was estimated at 5 cm. According to these hypotheses and based on a single factor ANOVA, a 0.05 alpha risk, and 90% power, 32 subjects per interval needed to be included, i.e. inclusion of 128 subjects per age group. Based on the assumption of 10% non-evaluable subjects, 141 subjects were therefore required per age group, i.e. a total number of 423 subjects.
                      This number was compatible with an expected proportion of 60% of children with a 10% precision (objective of exercise1), and an estimated kappa coefficient of 0.8 with 10% precision (exercise objective 5). The expected proportions for exercise objectives 2 and 3 required fewer subjects.
                      In the hospital setting (pediatric emergency department), it was decided to include 60 children for this pilot study, with an equal distribution over the three age groups (20 children/group) of 4–6 years, 7–9 years, and 10–12 years.

                      Statistical analyses

                      The qualitative variables were described as percentages with confidence intervals. The quantitative variables were described as mean ± standard deviation, or median and interquartile range. The study results were analyzed by age group (4-6 years; 7–9 years; 10–12 years) as in the study of Hicks (Hicks et al., 2001).
                      The specific objectives of exercises 1, 2, and 3 were analyzed using proportions and their confidence intervals.
                      Objective 1 was validated if the proportion of children at school confirming that the 6 faces scale expressed fear was at least 60%. The proportions observed in exercise 1 were compared with a theoretical distribution (25% for each category) using a Chi-squared test.
                      Objective 2 was validated if the proportion of children at school correctly ranking the faces expressing fear in descending order was at least 60% for kindergarten aged children and 80% for primary school aged children.
                      Objective 3 was validated if the proportion of children successfully identifying the faces expressing the most fear among the 15 pairs presented was at least 60% for pre-school aged children and 70% for the primary school aged children.
                      Objective 4: the means of the quantitative intervals of the scale levels were compared with the theoretical means using analysis of variance. In the event of overall significance, pairwise comparisons were performed by post-hoc comparisons (Fisher's predicted least-square difference [PLSD]).
                      Objective 5: the concordance between the evaluated levels of fear using the six-face scale for six situations between Phase 1 (Day 0) and Phase 2 (Day 15) was estimated by a weighted Cohen's kappa coefficient and its 95% confidence interval.
                      In the hospital setting, the proportion of children who successfully evaluated their fear using the six-face scale was presented with its 95% confidence interval. The concordance between the child's self-assessment of fear and the proxy assessment by caregiver 1 and caregiver 2 was determined by a weighted Cohen's kappa coefficient and its 95% confidence interval. The concordance of the proxy assessment of the child's fear between caregivers 1 and 2 was also determined by a weighted Cohen's kappa coefficient and its 95% confidence interval.
                      Results were considered to be moderate if they were between [0.40–0.60] and high if they were between [0.60–0.80] (
                      Landis and Koch, 1977
                      • Landis J.
                      • Koch G.
                      The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data.
                      Biometrics. 1977; 33: 159-174
                      https://doi.org/10.2307/2529310
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (52347)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ).

                      Results

                      A consensus was reached concerning the design and construction of the scale.

                      Results of psychometric assessment in the school setting

                      A total of 484 children aged from 4 to 12 years (mean age 7.57 years ±2.13) were included over 3 months in seven schools. Themean age per age group was 5.05 years (±0.77) in the 4–6 years group, 8.09 years (±0.76) in the 7–9 years group, and 10.29 years (±0.51) in the 10–12 years group.
                      All the children performed five exercises at phase 1. At phase 2, fifteen days later, exercise 5 was repeated.
                      A total of 467 children participated in the study until its end (17 children were sick).
                      Results of exercise 1:
                      Of the 483 children (99.79%) who performed this exercise, 57.64% (95%CI [53.22–62.21]; p < 0.0001) identified fear on the faces of the scale, 19.83% pain, 11.57% surprise and 10.74% sadness.
                      For each emotion, there was no significant difference according to the age group (Fig. 2). One child did not want to answer.
                      Fig. 2
                      Fig. 2Comparison of the proportions obtained for each emotion in the overall population and by age group compared to the theoretical intervals (chance).
                      • View Large Image
                      • Figure Viewer
                      • Download Hi-res image
                      • Download (PPT)
                      Results of exercise 2:
                      Of the 484 children (100%) who participated in this exercise, 324 (66.94%) successfully ranked the six faces in a descending order of fear intensity. The success rate was higher as age increased. For the 4–6 years: 47.2% of success (95%CI [33.24–50.87]), for the 7–9 years: 76.2% and 10–12 years: 78.7% of success (95%CI [71.27–80.40]). Of the 95 children who had mistaken two faces, 56 had inverted faces 5 and 6.
                      Results of exercise 3:
                      The exercise was carried out by 97.3% of children (98.2% (N = 162) aged 4–6 years, 98.1% (N = 202) aged 7–9 years and 94.7% (N = 107) aged 10–12 years).The face expressing the greatest fear was successfully distinguished by over 85% of the 4–6 years and over 92% for the 7–9 and 10–12 years for 14 pairs of faces. The last pair (Face 5 - Face 6) gave a score of 67.3% (N = 326) success in the overall population. Success was higher for this pair as age increased (4–6 years: 57.6%, 7–9 years: 71.8% and 10–12 years: 73.4%).
                      The results are presented in the overall population in Fig. 3.
                      Fig. 3
                      Fig. 3Percentage of success in discriminating faces expressing the greatest fear among 15 combinations in the overall population.
                      • View Large Image
                      • Figure Viewer
                      • Download Hi-res image
                      • Download (PPT)
                      Results of exercise 4:
                      All the children participated in this exercise. For each of the four faces, the number of children who placed the faces within the expected interval ranged from 432 (89.2%; face 3) to 447 (92.3%; face 5). The mean distance (in cm) for each face was within the expected interval. Additionally, the mean interval between the mean positions of the faces was 4.7 cm (Table 1).
                      Table 1Success (number, percentage and average distance) in positioning the faces within the expected interval [every 5 cm ±2].
                      NumberPercentage (%)Mean distance (cm)
                      Face 244090·96·3 +/− 2·6
                      Face 343289·210·8 +/− 2·6
                      Face 443990·715·9 +/− 2·5
                      Face 544792·320·4 +/− 2·04
                      • Open table in a new tab
                      Results of exercise 5:
                      This exercise was performed by 91.1% of children (91.5% (N = 151) of the 4–6 years group, 91.2% (N = 188) of the 7–9 years group and 90.3% (N = 102) of the 10–12 years group). Concordance scores are presented in (Table 2) for each fear situation and age group. Concordance scores were moderate to high.
                      Table 2Concordance between the two fear assessments (1st assessment and 15 days later) in 6 everyday situations assessed by the Weighed Kappa coefficient and its 95% confidence interval (wk [95% CI]) in the overall population and then in each age group.
                      Overall population4–6 years old7–9 years old10–12 years old
                      1. Fear of water (sea / pool)0·53 [0·43–0·59]0·46 [0·34–0·58]0·62
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·54–0·70]
                      0·62
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·54–0·70]
                      2. Fear of dark – darkness0·52 [0·46–0·58]0·40 [0·28–0·51]0·54 [0·45–0·63]0·66
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·56–0·75]
                      3. Fear of needle0·64
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·59–0·69]
                      0·62
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·53–0·72]
                      0·65
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·57–0·73]
                      0·60
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·50–0·70]
                      4. Fear of dogs0·58 [0·50–0·66]0·45 [0·29–0·61]0·58 [0·46–0·70]0·77
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·67–0·87]
                      5. Fear of the storm0·56 [0·50–0·62]0·46 [0·35–0·58]0·62
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·54–0·70]
                      0·58 [0·46–0·69]
                      6. Fear of spiders0·63
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·57–0·68]
                      0·50 [0·39–0·61]0·68
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·61–0·75]
                      0·72
                      ⁎
                      [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      [0·64–0·80]
                      low asterisk [0.60–0.80] = high agreement. [0.40–0.60] = moderate agreement.
                      • Open table in a new tab
                      The fear of needles had a high concordance score in each group (overall population 0.65 [0.60–0.70]).

                      Results of feasibility assessment in a hospital setting

                      Sixty children (mean age 7.82 years±2.49) were included in the pediatric emergency department. Forty-one (68.3%) children had already consulted in a pediatric emergency care department. Fifty-seven (95%) children managed to self-assess their fear level. There was little concordance between the self-assessment of fear by the children and the assessment by caregiver 1 (wk = 0.36) and that of caregiver 2 (wk = 0.44). The assessment by the two caregivers was highly concordant (wk = 0.69). The mean pain score was 3.28 (±2.56). The mean fear score was 1.75 (±2.62). These scores are weakly correlated (wk = 0.33) but in a statistically significant manner (p < 0.0112).

                      Discussion

                      The aim of our study was to develop and validate a self-assessment fear scale. Our results made it possible to validate the psychometric properties of the “Scary Scale” in a school setting and its feasibility ina hospital setting in children aged from 7 to 12 years. While the management of pain is now well codified, in particular with the use of pain assessment scales, scales for fear assessment are not widely used. Indeed, in 2011, McMurtry et al. tested a fear self-assessment scale, Children's Fear Scale (CFS), which was limited to a single context of procedural pain(
                      McMurtry, Noel, Chambers and McGrath, 2011
                      • McMurtry C.M.
                      • Noel M.
                      • Chambers C.T.
                      • McGrath P.J.
                      Children’s fear during procedural pain: Preliminary investigation of the Children’s fear scale.
                      Health Psychology: Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association. 2011; 30: 780-788
                      https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024817
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (166)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). The CFS is based on the Faces Anxiety Scale (FAS) (
                      McKinley, Coote and Stein-Parbury, 2003
                      • McKinley S.
                      • Coote K.
                      • Stein-Parbury J.
                      Development and testing of a faces scale for the assessment of anxiety in critically ill patients.
                      Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2003; 41: 73-79
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (83)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      McKinley and Madronio, 2008
                      • McKinley S.
                      • Madronio C.
                      Validity of the faces anxiety scale for the assessment of state anxiety in intensive care patients not receiving mechanical ventilation.
                      Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2008; 64: 503-507
                      https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.002
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (44)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ) to measure anxiety or fear. The Children's Fear Scale is not exclusively a fear scale. In McMurtry's study, the FAS was reduced to 5 faces to match with another scale (Children Anxiety and Pain Scale CAPS (
                      Kuttner and Lepage, 1989
                      • Kuttner L.
                      • Lepage T.
                      Face scales for the assessment of pediatric pain: A critical review.
                      Vol. 21(2). 1989: 198-209
                      • Google Scholar
                      )). The score of the CFS ranged from 0 to 4. It was not as widely distributed (221 citations in Google Scholar since 2011) as the issue warranted because of three main limitations: high graphic similarity with the Faces Pain Scale-Revised (FPS-R) (Hicks et al., 2001) that can induce caregiver confusion in its use, difficulties rating with a scale built with 5 faces instead of 6, resulting in different ratings (0–4 with CFS versus 0–10 with pain scales as FPS-r, VAS) and confusion of anxiety and fear that are two different concepts. Anxiety and fear are two closely related concepts and can be confused. Unlike fear, anxiety is a vague and unsettling feeling whose source is often nonspecific or unknown to the individual (
                      Ackley, Ladwig, Makic, Martinez-Kratz and Zanotti, 2019
                      • Ackley B.
                      • Ladwig G.
                      • Makic M.B.
                      • Martinez-Kratz M.
                      • Zanotti M.
                      Nursing diagnosis handbook. An evidence-based guide to planning care.
                      12th éd. Mosby, 2019
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). When asked, children are capable of stating the object of their fear (
                      Salmela, Salanterä and Aronen, 2009
                      • Salmela M.
                      • Salanterä S.
                      • Aronen E.
                      Child-reported hospital fears in 4 to 6-year-old children.
                      Pediatric Nursing. 2009; 35(5), 269-276: 303
                      • Google Scholar
                      ).
                      .
                      To avoid the same pitfall, we sharply differentiated the graphic design of our scale from those of pain scales. We have confirmed the choice of a scale with faces since, for the assessment of pain, children seem to have a preference for scales with faces (
                      Brahimi et al., 2022
                      • Brahimi L.
                      • Wood C.
                      • Benkerrou M.
                      • Vieyra M.
                      • Bourillon A.
                      • Vilmer E.
                      Prospective study of the use of two pain assessment tools the visual analogue scale (VAS) and the face scale (FPS) in school-aged children with sickle cell disease-Pediadol.
                      https://pediadol.org/etude-prospective-de-lutilisation-de-deux-outils-devaluation-de-la-douleur-lechelle-visuelle-analogue-eva-et-lechelle-des-visages-fps-chez-lenfant-drepanocytaire-dage-scolaire/
                      Date: 2022
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). This is because the measurements with these scales are concrete and suitable for young children who do not need to know how to count to use them (
                      McGrath, Latimer, Finley and Chambers, 2009
                      • McGrath P.J.
                      • Latimer M.
                      • Finley G.A.
                      • Chambers C.T.
                      Measurement of pain in children.
                      Pain Research & Management: The Journal of the Canadian Pain Society. 2009; 14: 11
                      https://doi.org/10.1093/ptj/84.6.560
                      • Crossref
                      • Scopus (6)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). Using a self-assessment scale would serve to identify, objectivate fear and make it real. It is known that children can answer yes or no to familiar objects (
                      Fritzley and Lee, 2003
                      • Fritzley V.H.
                      • Lee K.
                      Do young children always say yes to yes-no questions? A metadevelopmental study of the affirmation bias.
                      Child Development. 2003; 74: 1297-1313
                      https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00608
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (151)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ), but, what about an emotion? For information, fear is a distressing emotion caused by an impending danger or pain, whether the threat is real or imagined (
                      Ackley, Ladwig, Makic, Martinez-Kratz and Zanotti, 2019
                      • Ackley B.
                      • Ladwig G.
                      • Makic M.B.
                      • Martinez-Kratz M.
                      • Zanotti M.
                      Nursing diagnosis handbook. An evidence-based guide to planning care.
                      12th éd. Mosby, 2019
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). We thus hypothesized that this method would be better than asking the children, “do you feel fear? Yes or no?” because some children would lack the abstraction capability to numerically evaluate fear (
                      Siegler, DeLoache and Eisenberg, 2010
                      • Siegler R.S.
                      • DeLoache J.S.
                      • Eisenberg N.
                      How children develop.
                      3rd ed. Worth Publishers, 2010
                      • Google Scholar
                      ).
                      We designed a fear scale, based on the work by Ekman (
                      Ekman and Friesen, 2002
                      • Ekman P.
                      • Friesen W.V.
                      Investigator’s guide to the facial action coding system (FACS).
                      2002
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Ekman and Friesen, 2003
                      • Ekman P.
                      • Friesen W.V.
                      Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial expressions (Malor books).
                      2003
                      • Google Scholar
                      ) with six faces in order to be consistent with the pain assessment scales. A common metric system enabled the use of conventional and universal language for evaluating the scores (Hicks et al., 2001). To achieve its wide use, the Scary Scale is neutral in terms of gender, ethnic origin and emotional elements such as tears and smiling. The absence of tears and smiles gives better assessments (
                      Tomlinson, Von Baeyer, Stinson and Sung, 2010
                      • Tomlinson D.
                      • Von Baeyer C.L.
                      • Stinson J.N.
                      • Sung L.
                      A systematic review of faces scales for the self-report of pain intensity in children.
                      Pediatrics. 2010; 126: e1168-e1198
                      https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-1609
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (359)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ) and avoids certain biases such as under-assessment (
                      HAS, 2022
                      • HAS
                      Hight authority of health (French:Haute Autorité de Santé) - Evaluation and strategies for the management of acute pain in ambulatory care in children aged 1 month to 15 years.
                      https://www.has-sante.fr/portail/jcms/c_272030/fr/evaluation-et-strategies-de-prise-en-charge-de-la-douleur-aigue-en-ambulatoire-chez-l-enfant-de-1-mois-a-15-ans
                      Date: 2022
                      • Google Scholar
                      ).
                      We avoided the use of pink or blue colors, which, in the collective subconscious, symbolize girls and boys, respectively. In European culture and more widely in Western culture, the color green has two dimensions: a negative one suggesting fear and a positive one suggesting calm, nature and reassurance (
                      Pastoureau, 2014
                      • Pastoureau M.
                      Green: The history of a color.
                      Princeton University Press, 2014
                      • Google Scholar
                      ). The green color of our faces suggested fear without triggering it.
                      Validating the psychometric properties of a scale is a tremendous process.To achieve this aim, we used the same criteria for validating the psychometric properties as those used to validate the Face Pain Scale in the Bieri et al. study (Bieri et al., 1990) (FPS-R is the Gold Standard for assessing children's pain score).
                      We modeled our study on five properties. Each exercise provided a supplemental element of validation.
                      • -
                        The purpose of exercise 1 was to confirm that the graphics of our scale really express fear. In this exercise, like Bieri et al. who has validated with 57.9%, we had set an objective that 60% of the children would identify fear among three other emotions with which it can be confused: pain, sadness and surprise. Our results were very close to the objective (57.64%). The responses of the children were not due to chance (p < 0.0001) but were consistent with their choice.
                      • -
                        The purpose of exercise 2 was to assess the intensity gradient in the expression of fear on the six faces. In this exercise, similar to Bieri et al., we had set an objective that 60% of the 4–6 year-olds and 80% of the 7–9 and 10–12 year-olds would rank the faces in descending order. Our results were very close to our objective for the 7–9 and the 10–12 year-olds. The 4–6 year-olds did not reach the set objective, particularly with regard to distinguishing intermediate nuances (
                        Besenski and Forsyth, 2007
                        • Besenski L.J.
                        • Forsyth S.J.
                        Screening young children for their ability to use self-report pain scales.
                        Vol. 9(1). 2007: 7
                        • Google Scholar
                        ;
                        Decruynaere, Thonnard and Plaghki, 2009
                        • Decruynaere C.
                        • Thonnard J.-L.
                        • Plaghki L.
                        How many response levels do children distinguish on faces scales for pain assessment?.
                        European Journal of Pain (London, England). 2009; 13: 641-648
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.07.004
                        • Crossref
                        • PubMed
                        • Scopus (33)
                        • Google Scholar
                        ;
                        Emmott et al., 2017
                        • Emmott A.S.
                        • West N.
                        • Zhou G.
                        • Dunsmuir D.
                        • Montgomery C.J.
                        • Lauder G.R.
                        • Von Baeyer C.L.
                        Validity of simplified versus standard self-report measures of pain intensity in preschool-aged children undergoing venipuncture.
                        The Journal of Pain: Official Journal of the American Pain Society. 2017; 18: 564-573
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.12.015
                        • Abstract
                        • Full Text
                        • Full Text PDF
                        • PubMed
                        • Scopus (25)
                        • Google Scholar
                        ) due to their intellectual maturity level (
                        Besenski and Forsyth, 2007
                        • Besenski L.J.
                        • Forsyth S.J.
                        Screening young children for their ability to use self-report pain scales.
                        Vol. 9(1). 2007: 7
                        • Google Scholar
                        ;
                        Chan and Von Baeyer, 2016
                        • Chan J.Y.-C.
                        • Von Baeyer C.L.
                        Cognitive developmental influences on the ability of preschool-aged children to self-report their pain intensity.
                        Pain. 2016; 157: 997-1001
                        https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000476
                        • Crossref
                        • PubMed
                        • Scopus (36)
                        • Google Scholar
                        ;
                        Siegler, DeLoache and Eisenberg, 2010
                        • Siegler R.S.
                        • DeLoache J.S.
                        • Eisenberg N.
                        How children develop.
                        3rd ed. Worth Publishers, 2010
                        • Google Scholar
                        ). The failure to achieve this objective led us to exclude the 4–6 year-olds from the rest of the discussion.
                      • -
                        The purpose of exercise 3 was to assess that faces were different from each other, specifically when they follow one after the other (e.g.: Face 1 - Face 2; Face 2 - Face 3, etc.). In this exercise, we had set an objective that 70% of the 7–9 and 10–12 year-olds would discriminate the face expressing the greatest fear for each pair. The objective was achieved.
                      • -
                        The purpose of exercise 4 was to determine whether the intensity between the faces increased equally. In this exercise, we had set an objective that the children would have positioned the faces in the right place (>89%) and equally (4.7 cm). We consider that the objective of 5 cm is validated. Our scale is well-calibrated.
                      • -
                        The purpose of exercise 5 was to assess the reproducibility of our scale, i.e. the ability of a person to self-assess in the same way at two different times. In this exercise, we had set an objective that the reproducibility of assessment of real life situations triggering fear would be moderate to high for 7–9 and 10–12 year-olds. This objective was achieved.
                      Our scale is hence validated through the 7–12 year-olds' validation of the five psychometric properties.
                      With regard to the evaluation of the clinical application and contextualization of the scale in the hospital, among the children who were recruited in the pediatric emergency department (N = 60), 95% (N = 57) were able to identify their fear level (expected threshold of 80%). 5% (N = 3) were unable to self-assess the level of fear or of pain. This difficulty was attributed to the immaturity of these children and not to problems understanding the instructions, as shown previously with the psychometric validation. Even though the 4–6 year-olds did not succeed in all the exercises, some results remain very interesting. The fear self-assessment scale may still be used depending on the degree of intellectual maturity. A simplified version of the Scary Scale would need to be developed for this specific age group.
                      We also wanted to study objectively the difference between children's self-assessment and caregivers' assessment of children's fear (subjectivity).The proxy assessments of the child's level of fear, carried out by two independent caregivers, were concordant with each other. There was little concordance, however, between each caregiver and the child's self-assessment. Using our scale, the proxy assessment of fear by the caregivers thus does not reflect the child's feelings. It would therefore be preferable for the child to perform a self-assessment of fear by themselves. We could infer that our scale is clearly a fear self-assessment scale and not a proxy assessment scale. It would be interesting, as for the pain scale, to develop a proxy assessment fear scale.
                      To be well-accepted and used in routine care, a scale must be approved by both children and caregivers. Our scale is play-based and easy to use by children. The feedback of pediatric caregivers after using this scale showed a high level of compliance.

                      Limitations and perspectives

                      The 4 to 6-year-olds failed to reach the set objectives, though some of the results remain very interesting. The fear self-assessment scale may still be used depending on the degree of intellectual maturity, or, alternatively, we can also consider a new study for validating a simplified version of the Scary Scale aimed at providing an age-appropriate fear self-assessment tool for these children.
                      As our objective was to validate the Scary Scale, in this study, we wanted to specifically address the topic of fear as there are still few studies on fear in children. We only sought to study the link between fear and pain at admission to the pediatric emergency department. We did not consider anxiety in this pilot study (n = 60 children). Pain is well-evaluated with validated scales, while anxiety and fear are not systematically identified and treated in routine. Further research could study more specifically the link between pain - fear and anxiety in children undergoing care situations. To assess the universal potential of the scale, studies with larger numbers of participants and in different areas are needed.
                      In the hospital, children may be confronted with many situations of care (venipuncture, suture, dressings, radiological exams, surgical procedures, etc.) that can cause several mixed emotions. Pain, anxiety and fear are the most frequently experienced by the children.
                      If caregivers fail to assess children's fear, they remain trapped in their fear. This is a matter of true concern, because underestimating fear can lead to complicated situations where caregivers use restraints (
                      Brenner, 2007
                      • Brenner Maria
                      Child restraint in the acute setting of pediatric nursing: an extraordinarily stressful event.
                      Issues Compr Pediatr Nurs. 2007; 30: 29-37https://doi.org/10.1080/01460860701366658
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (24)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ;
                      Svendsen, Pedersen, Moen and Bjørk, 2017
                      • Svendsen E.J.
                      • Pedersen R.
                      • Moen A.
                      • Bjørk I.T.
                      Exploring perspectives on restraint during medical procedures in paediatric care: A qualitative interview study with nurses and physicians.
                      International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being. 2017; 12: 1363623
                      https://doi.org/10.1080/17482631.2017.1363623
                      • Crossref
                      • PubMed
                      • Scopus (17)
                      • Google Scholar
                      ) and misuse pain killers. These situations are very traumatic to the children, their families and also the caregivers. It is essential to establish a relationship of trust in the child – parents – caregivers triad by taking a few minutes before any care to meet the child, explain the procedure and assess the child's degree of fear with the Scary Scale. The caregiver can then use appropriate play materials to provide additional information. The care will be made less dramatic and the child will become actor of the situation. If the child is cooperative, the use of restraint will no be required.
                      For all these reasons, we propose the use of the Scary Scale in combination with pain assessment as soon as the caregivers identify behavioral components such as crying, screaming, avoidance, etc. This combined assessment, will help to better differentiate between the diagnosis of fear and pain and allow nurses to adapt their medication and distraction strategies.

                      Funding

                      This study received funding from the French Ministry of Health. Funding for this study was used for the printing of all study materials, nursing time for school and hospital inclusions, data collection, management, and analysis.

                      CRediT authorship contribution statement

                      S. Thurillet: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Funding acquisition. C. Bahans: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. C. Wood: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. S. Bougnard: Conceptualization, Investigation. A. Labrunie: Software, Formal analysis. V. Messager: Investigation. J. Toniolo: Writing – review & editing. P. Beloni: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Funding acquisition. L. Fourcade: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

                      Declaration of Competing Interest

                      All authors have no conflict to declare.

                      Acknowledgments

                      We wish to thank Mr. Coiffe, who produced the graphics for the Scary Scale.
                      We would like to give special thanks to Mr. Luc Valade, retired school principal, who recruited the schools for this study, facilitating the collaboration between the hospital and school settings. We would like to express our gratitude to Coralie Fluteau, Evelyne Gayet-Boulon and Oumar Seck, who actively participated inthe inclusions in the schools and in the data management.
                      We are grateful to Professor Guigonis for his support, review, advice and critical eye.
                      And we would like to express our gratitude to the pediatric emergency departments of Limoges for the inclusions in the hospital setting.

                      Appendix A. Supplementary data

                      • Download .pdf (.67 MB)
                        Help with pdf files

                        Supplementary material

                      References

                        • Ackley B.
                        • Ladwig G.
                        • Makic M.B.
                        • Martinez-Kratz M.
                        • Zanotti M.
                        Nursing diagnosis handbook. An evidence-based guide to planning care.
                        12th éd. Mosby, 2019
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Besenski L.J.
                        • Forsyth S.J.
                        Screening young children for their ability to use self-report pain scales.
                        Vol. 9(1). 2007: 7
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Beyer J.E.
                        • Turner S.B.
                        • Jones L.
                        • Young L.
                        • Onikul R.
                        • Bohaty B.
                        The alternate forms reliability of the Oucher pain scale.
                        Pain Management Nursing: Official Journal of the American Society of Pain Management Nurses. 2005; 6: 10-17
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmn.2004.11.001
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (33)
                        • PubMed
                        • Abstract
                        • Full Text
                        • Full Text PDF
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Beyer Judith E
                        • et al.
                        The creation, validation, and continuing development of the oucher: A measure of pain intensity in children.
                        Journal of Pediatric Nursing: Nursing Care of Children and Families. 1992; 7: 335-346https://doi.org/10.5555/uri:pii:0882596392900426
                        View in Article
                        • PubMed
                        • Abstract
                        • Full Text PDF
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Bieri D
                        • et al.
                        The Faces Pain Scale for the self-assessment of the severity of pain experienced by children: development, initial validation, and preliminary investigation for ratio scale properties.
                        Pain. 1990; 41: 139-150https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(90)90018-9
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (875)
                        • PubMed
                        • Abstract
                        • Full Text PDF
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Binet A.
                        Fear in children.
                        Vol. 2. 1895: 223-254
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Brahimi L.
                        • Wood C.
                        • Benkerrou M.
                        • Vieyra M.
                        • Bourillon A.
                        • Vilmer E.
                        Prospective study of the use of two pain assessment tools the visual analogue scale (VAS) and the face scale (FPS) in school-aged children with sickle cell disease-Pediadol.
                        (Retrieved from)
                        https://pediadol.org/etude-prospective-de-lutilisation-de-deux-outils-devaluation-de-la-douleur-lechelle-visuelle-analogue-eva-et-lechelle-des-visages-fps-chez-lenfant-drepanocytaire-dage-scolaire/
                        Date: 2022
                        (Accessed August 30, 2017)
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Brenner Maria
                        Child restraint in the acute setting of pediatric nursing: an extraordinarily stressful event.
                        Issues Compr Pediatr Nurs. 2007; 30: 29-37https://doi.org/10.1080/01460860701366658
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (24)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Chan J.Y.-C.
                        • Von Baeyer C.L.
                        Cognitive developmental influences on the ability of preschool-aged children to self-report their pain intensity.
                        Pain. 2016; 157: 997-1001
                        https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000476
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (36)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Copper-Royer B.
                        Fear of the wolf, fear of everything.
                        Albin Michel, 2013
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Dantzer R.
                        The psychobiology of fear.
                        Communications. 1993; 57: 25-34
                        https://doi.org/10.3406/comm.1993.1864
                        View in Article
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Decruynaere C.
                        • Thonnard J.-L.
                        • Plaghki L.
                        How many response levels do children distinguish on faces scales for pain assessment?.
                        European Journal of Pain (London, England). 2009; 13: 641-648
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.07.004
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (33)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Ekman P.
                        • Friesen W.V.
                        Investigator’s guide to the facial action coding system (FACS).
                        2002
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Ekman P.
                        • Friesen W.V.
                        Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial expressions (Malor books).
                        2003
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Emmott A.S.
                        • West N.
                        • Zhou G.
                        • Dunsmuir D.
                        • Montgomery C.J.
                        • Lauder G.R.
                        • Von Baeyer C.L.
                        Validity of simplified versus standard self-report measures of pain intensity in preschool-aged children undergoing venipuncture.
                        The Journal of Pain: Official Journal of the American Pain Society. 2017; 18: 564-573
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.12.015
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (25)
                        • PubMed
                        • Abstract
                        • Full Text
                        • Full Text PDF
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Foster R.L.
                        • Park J.
                        An integrative review of literature examining psychometric properties of instruments measuring anxiety or fear in hospitalized children.
                        Pain Management Nursing: Official Journal of the American Society of Pain Management Nurses. 2012; 13: 94-106
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmn.2011.06.006
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (33)
                        • PubMed
                        • Abstract
                        • Full Text
                        • Full Text PDF
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Fritzley V.H.
                        • Lee K.
                        Do young children always say yes to yes-no questions? A metadevelopmental study of the affirmation bias.
                        Child Development. 2003; 74: 1297-1313
                        https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00608
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (151)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Garber S.
                        • Garber M.
                        • Spizman R.
                        Your child's fears. How to help him/her overcome them.
                        Odile Jacob, 1997
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Garra G.
                        • Singer A.J.
                        • Taira B.R.
                        • Chohan J.
                        • Cardoz H.
                        • Chisena E.
                        • Thode H.C.
                        Validation of the Wong-baker FACES pain rating scale in pediatric emergency department patients.
                        Academic Emergency Medicine: Official Journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine. 2010; 17: 50-54
                        https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2009.00620.x
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (326)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • HAS
                        Hight authority of health (French:Haute Autorité de Santé) - Evaluation and strategies for the management of acute pain in ambulatory care in children aged 1 month to 15 years.
                        (Retrieved from)
                        https://www.has-sante.fr/portail/jcms/c_272030/fr/evaluation-et-strategies-de-prise-en-charge-de-la-douleur-aigue-en-ambulatoire-chez-l-enfant-de-1-mois-a-15-ans
                        Date: 2022
                        (Accessed April 3, 2019)
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Henderson V.
                        Basic principles of nursing care.
                        American Nurses Association, 1997
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Hicks Carrie L
                        • von Baeyer Carl L
                        • Spafford Pamela A
                        • van Korlaar Inez
                        • Goodenough Belinda
                        The Faces Pain Scale-Revised: toward a common metric in pediatric pain measurement.
                        pain. 2001; 93: 173-183https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00314-1
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (1350)
                        • PubMed
                        • Abstract
                        • Full Text
                        • Full Text PDF
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Huskisson E.C.
                        Measurement of pain.
                        Lancet (London, England). 1974; 2: 1127-1131
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(74)90884-8
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (3163)
                        • PubMed
                        • Abstract
                        • Google Scholar
                        • International Association for the Study of Pain
                        IASP terminology - Pain definition.
                        2022
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Kuttner L.
                        • Lepage T.
                        Face scales for the assessment of pediatric pain: A critical review.
                        Vol. 21(2). 1989: 198-209
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Landis J.
                        • Koch G.
                        The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data.
                        Biometrics. 1977; 33: 159-174
                        https://doi.org/10.2307/2529310
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (52347)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Marchand S.
                        The phenomenon of pain. Understanding for better care.
                        Masson, 1998
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • McGrath P.J.
                        • Latimer M.
                        • Finley G.A.
                        • Chambers C.T.
                        Measurement of pain in children.
                        Pain Research & Management: The Journal of the Canadian Pain Society. 2009; 14: 11
                        https://doi.org/10.1093/ptj/84.6.560
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (6)
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • McKinley S.
                        • Coote K.
                        • Stein-Parbury J.
                        Development and testing of a faces scale for the assessment of anxiety in critically ill patients.
                        Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2003; 41: 73-79
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (83)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • McKinley S.
                        • Madronio C.
                        Validity of the faces anxiety scale for the assessment of state anxiety in intensive care patients not receiving mechanical ventilation.
                        Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2008; 64: 503-507
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.002
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (44)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • McMurtry C.M.
                        • Noel M.
                        • Chambers C.T.
                        • McGrath P.J.
                        Children’s fear during procedural pain: Preliminary investigation of the Children’s fear scale.
                        Health Psychology: Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association. 2011; 30: 780-788
                        https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024817
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (166)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Pastoureau M.
                        Green: The history of a color.
                        Princeton University Press, 2014
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Petitcollin C.
                        Emotions, user's guide.
                        Jouvence, 2003
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Pilon C.
                        Relieve the child's pain.
                        Hôpital Sainte Justine, 1999
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Salmela M.
                        • Salanterä S.
                        • Aronen E.
                        Child-reported hospital fears in 4 to 6-year-old children.
                        Pediatric Nursing. 2009; 35(5), 269-276: 303
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Siegler R.S.
                        • DeLoache J.S.
                        • Eisenberg N.
                        How children develop.
                        3rd ed. Worth Publishers, 2010
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Streiner D.L.
                        • Norman G.R.
                        Health measurement scales. A practical guide to their development and use.
                        3rd éd. Oxford University Press, 2003
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Svendsen E.J.
                        • Pedersen R.
                        • Moen A.
                        • Bjørk I.T.
                        Exploring perspectives on restraint during medical procedures in paediatric care: A qualitative interview study with nurses and physicians.
                        International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being. 2017; 12: 1363623
                        https://doi.org/10.1080/17482631.2017.1363623
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (17)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Testu F.
                        • Bréchon G.
                        • Clarisse R.
                        • Fontaine R.
                        • Le Floc’h, N.
                        Life rhythms and school rhythms.
                        Elsevier Masson, 2008
                        View in Article
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Tomlinson D.
                        • Von Baeyer C.L.
                        • Stinson J.N.
                        • Sung L.
                        A systematic review of faces scales for the self-report of pain intensity in children.
                        Pediatrics. 2010; 126: e1168-e1198
                        https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-1609
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (359)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Von Baeyer C.L.
                        Children’s self-reports of pain intensity: Scale selection, limitations and interpretation.
                        Pain Research & Management: The Journal of the Canadian Pain Society. 2006; 11: 157-162
                        https://doi.org/10.1155/2006/197616
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (300)
                        • PubMed
                        • Crossref
                        • Google Scholar
                        • Yeh C.-H.
                        Development and validation of the Asian version of the oucher: A pain intensity scale for children.
                        The Journal of Pain: Official Journal of the American Pain Society. 2005; 6: 526-534
                        https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2005.03.002
                        View in Article
                        • Scopus (20)
                        • PubMed
                        • Abstract
                        • Full Text
                        • Full Text PDF
                        • Google Scholar

                      Article info

                      Publication history

                      Published online: March 14, 2022
                      Accepted: February 23, 2022
                      Received in revised form: February 21, 2022
                      Received: October 21, 2021

                      Identification

                      DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2022.02.020

                      Copyright

                      © 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc.

                      User license

                      Creative Commons Attribution – NonCommercial – NoDerivs (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0) |
                      How you can reuse Information Icon
                      Creative Commons Attribution – NonCommercial – NoDerivs (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)

                      Permitted

                      For non-commercial purposes:

                      • Read, print & download
                      • Redistribute or republish the final article
                      • Text & data mine
                      • Translate the article (private use only, not for distribution)
                      • Reuse portions or extracts from the article in other works

                      Not Permitted

                      • Sell or re-use for commercial purposes
                      • Distribute translations or adaptations of the article

                      Elsevier's open access license policy

                      ScienceDirect

                      Access this article on ScienceDirect
                        Hide CaptionDownloadSee figure in article
                        Toggle Thumbstrip
                          • View Large Image
                          • Download Hi-res image
                          • Download .PPT

                          Related Articles

                            Hide CaptionDownloadSee figure in Article
                            Toggle Thumbstrip
                              • Download Hi-res image
                              • Download .PPT
                              • Home
                              • Articles & Issues
                              • Current Issue
                              • List of Issues
                              • Covid-19 Information
                              • For Authors
                              • Permissions
                              • Researcher Academy
                              • Journal Info
                              • About the Journal
                              • Abstracting/Indexing
                              • Activate Online Access
                              • Career Opportunities
                              • Contact Information
                              • Editorial Board
                              • Info for Advertisers
                              • Pricing
                              • Reprints
                              • New Content Alerts
                              • Subscribe
                              • More Periodicals
                              • Find a Periodical
                              • Go to Product Catalog

                              The content on this site is intended for healthcare professionals.



                              We use cookies to help provide and enhance our service and tailor content. To update your cookie settings, please visit the Cookie Preference Center for this site.
                              Copyright © 2023 Elsevier Inc. except certain content provided by third parties.

                              • Privacy Policy  
                              • Terms and Conditions  
                              • Accessibility  
                              • Help & Contact

                              RELX